全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5261篇 |
免费 | 67篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
教育 | 3870篇 |
科学研究 | 434篇 |
各国文化 | 62篇 |
体育 | 314篇 |
综合类 | 1篇 |
文化理论 | 41篇 |
信息传播 | 609篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 53篇 |
2020年 | 81篇 |
2019年 | 146篇 |
2018年 | 222篇 |
2017年 | 203篇 |
2016年 | 177篇 |
2015年 | 126篇 |
2014年 | 138篇 |
2013年 | 1174篇 |
2012年 | 113篇 |
2011年 | 138篇 |
2010年 | 119篇 |
2009年 | 114篇 |
2008年 | 133篇 |
2007年 | 125篇 |
2006年 | 106篇 |
2005年 | 99篇 |
2004年 | 91篇 |
2003年 | 80篇 |
2002年 | 86篇 |
2001年 | 68篇 |
2000年 | 68篇 |
1999年 | 62篇 |
1998年 | 60篇 |
1997年 | 65篇 |
1996年 | 50篇 |
1995年 | 50篇 |
1994年 | 54篇 |
1993年 | 52篇 |
1992年 | 58篇 |
1991年 | 57篇 |
1990年 | 50篇 |
1989年 | 59篇 |
1988年 | 48篇 |
1987年 | 29篇 |
1986年 | 55篇 |
1985年 | 59篇 |
1984年 | 51篇 |
1983年 | 45篇 |
1982年 | 46篇 |
1981年 | 28篇 |
1980年 | 41篇 |
1979年 | 38篇 |
1978年 | 38篇 |
1977年 | 36篇 |
1976年 | 35篇 |
1967年 | 24篇 |
1939年 | 23篇 |
1937年 | 41篇 |
1936年 | 40篇 |
排序方式: 共有5331条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
O. G. 《Sportwissenschaft》2003,33(2):235
Deutscher Sportbund
Hermann-Altrock-Stipendium Ausschreibung 2003 相似文献32.
33.
The validity of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) (Kaufman & Kaufman, 1983) was investigated with a sample of preschool-age children. The sample consisted of 50 subjects aged 21/2 through 4 who were distributed roughly equally by age, sex, and race according to the 1980 census. The validity of the K-ABC was examined using the Slosson Intelligence Test (SIT) (Slosson, 1982) and the Classroom Behavior Inventory-Preschool Form (CBI) (Schaefer & Edgerton, 1978) as criterion measures. The SIT and CBI-Preschool Form Verbal Intelligence Scale were found to correlate significantly (p<.01) with the global scales of the K-ABC. The K-ABC did not differentiate between age or sex groups, but blacks scored lower than whites. The findings are supportive of previous validity studies (Kaufman & Kaufman, 1983) and two-factor theories of intelligence (e.g., Das, Kirby, & Jarman, 1975). 相似文献
34.
Twenty-eight seventh and eighth grade students were randomly assigned to two treatments and a control as they entered the Florida State Museum. All subjects were given set induction materials saying in effect: (1) Treatment-Study the wall panel which is part of the cave exhibit and describes the many biologic relationships and organisms in the cave at the cave entrance. Continue into the cave and study the habitat. When you exit the cave you will be given a test to see how much you learned; (2) Treatment II-Enter the cave and study that habitat. When you exit at the other end, study the wall panel which is part of the cave exit and describes the many biologic relationships and organisms in the cave. After studying this panel you will be given a test to see how much you learned; (3) Control-You will take a walk through exhibits depicting a variety of Florida Habitats and later see a film on the Habitats of Florida that are represented in the museum. Then you will be given a test to determine how much you know about one type of habitat. Both treatments and the control spent equivalent time at their tasks and were confronted with either the cave exhibit as described or the equivalent control experiences. A 25 item criterion test was designed to measure the acquisition of conceptual and factual knowledge and specific attentional factors relative to both the cave and the instructional panel. A Kr-20 indicated that this instrument had a reliability coefficient of 0.80. It was anticipated that the panel, used as an attention directing and controlling device prior to entering the cave (Treatment I) or upon exiting the cave (Treatment II), would function as a forward-shaping or backward review prompting adjunct, hence both should be significantly more effective than the control. This was confirmed (df 2,28; F 8.09) p < 0.01. It was also expected that the forward shaping panel would be more effective than the backward review panel in the tradition of inserted questions in prose research. Although the differences were in this direction, they were not significant (p≤ 0.10). 相似文献
35.
The international perceptions of Japanese young adults were explored in 172 professional education students in two Tokyo colleges. They classified 30 nations according to their “current” and “future” affinity to Japan. Little difference was detected in the Q-sorts between institutions arid sexes, while the “current” and “future” sorts correlated .96 with each other. In cluster analyses, many two-nation units (e.g., Czechoslovakia-Yugoslavia) were found to be the building blocks for the overall pattern. The total configuration consisted of three broad groups: mostly Western European nations, problematical nations (China, Russia, Israel, Egypt, etc.), and remote and neutral nations. Structurally, the results were similar to those for American young adults, but the contents were different. The notion of cultural metastereotypes thus received tentative support. 相似文献
36.
37.
38.
39.
The popularity and viewership of Nollywood films have expanded across African and Western cities especially those with considerable Diaspora population. This study seeks to investigate whether watching Nollywood films is likely to lead to cross-cultural transmission of Nigeria’s cultural heritage to audience members in South Africa. Data for the study were collected using observation and surveys with Likert-type scales involving participants from a South African college. Results show that watching Nollywood films was significant in increasing participants’ knowledge and appreciation of Nigerian culture. Observed patterns indicate that a small portion of participants could mimic or reproduce some common Nigerian expressions and exclamations while a negligible percentage of participants could actually speak a smattering of Igbo, one of Nigeria’s three major languages. 相似文献
40.
Todd L. Sandel Richard Buttny Mary Varghese 《Journal of Intercultural Communication Research》2019,48(1):52-71
Students were linked virtually across three contexts: U.S., Malaysia, and China. Differences emerged in how student-created messages were constructed and interpreted; these impacted perceived relational affiliation. Messages constructed by students in Malaysia and China exhibited casual talk, greater self-disclosure, requests for personal information, and greater use of emoticons/emoji. Interactions were perceived as informal, friendly, and positive. U.S. students’ messages often showed institutional talk, less self-disclosure, and more attention to the instructor-assigned task. Asian-U.S. student pairs perceived their interactions as formal, less friendly, and less positive. This study shows how online technologies may be afforded and shaped by culture and interaction. 相似文献