首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   15754篇
  免费   143篇
  国内免费   12篇
教育   10986篇
科学研究   1684篇
各国文化   230篇
体育   1024篇
综合类   8篇
文化理论   150篇
信息传播   1827篇
  2020年   153篇
  2019年   253篇
  2018年   269篇
  2017年   363篇
  2016年   302篇
  2015年   218篇
  2014年   301篇
  2013年   3398篇
  2012年   285篇
  2011年   274篇
  2010年   236篇
  2009年   276篇
  2008年   311篇
  2007年   295篇
  2006年   274篇
  2005年   248篇
  2004年   233篇
  2003年   225篇
  2002年   204篇
  2001年   182篇
  2000年   220篇
  1999年   187篇
  1998年   145篇
  1997年   149篇
  1996年   165篇
  1995年   145篇
  1994年   173篇
  1993年   176篇
  1992年   206篇
  1991年   222篇
  1990年   221篇
  1989年   227篇
  1988年   190篇
  1987年   197篇
  1986年   209篇
  1985年   212篇
  1984年   215篇
  1983年   233篇
  1982年   175篇
  1981年   189篇
  1980年   166篇
  1979年   203篇
  1978年   228篇
  1977年   166篇
  1976年   153篇
  1975年   150篇
  1974年   115篇
  1973年   138篇
  1972年   122篇
  1970年   124篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
122.
This paper addresses the parallel between the changes in students' and teachers' learning advocated by constructivist science educators. It begins with a summary of the epistemology of constructivism and uses a vignette drawn from a set of case studies to explore the impact of a constructivist science in‐service programme on an experienced and formal elementary science teacher. Judged by constructivist standards, the teacher described in the vignette makes very little progress. The irony of applying a constructivist critique to his work, however, is that it fails to treat the teachers' imperfect knowledge of teaching with the same respect as constructivists treat students' imperfect learning of science. The remainder of the paper explores this constructivist paradox, and suggests that‐like students' knowledge of science‐teachers' knowledge of constructivist science teaching is likely to grow through slow and gradual re‐formation of their established understanding of classroom theory and practice.  相似文献   
123.
Abstract

Correspondence study represents the first and most persistent distance education format in American universities. Later called independent study, it enabled universities to disseminate instruction far beyond their campuses. Yet, national‐level leadership provided by the National University Continuing Education Association (NUEA) and its divisions has been relatively restrained. In contrast, leadership in the private correspondence school sector has been assertive, and sometimes even aggressive. The NUEA and its members shunned this approach, choosing instead to lead by persuasion and example. The NUEA developed standards of practice concerned primarily with replicating on‐campus teaching styles and values, rather than the promotion of distance education. With the abolition of its division structure, the NUEA's successor, the University Continuing Education Association (UCEA), has opted out of a leadership role in independent study. This paper concludes that the NUEA's initial attempt at leadership in distance education— while reasoned and principled—contained flaws that made failure inevitable.  相似文献   
124.
125.
The purpose of this study was to determine whether teacher enthusiasm and student notetaking activity would interact to produce significant differences in students’ learning from lecture. Three modes of student learning activity (listen only, notes, notes plus review) were crossed with two levels of lecturer enthusiasm (less enthusiastic, more enthusiastic) in a 3X2 MANOVA design. Dependent variables were simple recall, complex recall, and recognition of lecture content. Results showed a significant interaction for simple recall. Also, a significant main effect was obtained for learning activity. Findings are discussed with respect to how learning activity and teacher behavior can function jointly in student processing of information. Suggestions for future research are given in light of limitations to the present study.  相似文献   
126.
127.
128.
129.
130.
Although creativity and expertise are related, they are nonetheless very different things. Expertise does not usually require creativity, but creativity generally does require a certain level of expertise. There are similarities in the relationships of both expertise and creativity to domains, however. Research has shown that just as expertise in one domain does not predict expertise in other, unrelated domains, creativity in one domain does not predict creativity in other, unrelated domains. People may be expert, and people may be creative, in many domains, or they may be expert, or creative, in few domains or none at all, and one cannot simply transfer expertise, or creativity, from one domain to another, unrelated domain. The domain specificity of creativity matters crucially for creativity training, creativity assessment, creativity research, and creativity theory. The domain specificity of creativity also means that interdisciplinary thinking, interdisciplinary collaboration, and interdisciplinary creativity are even more important than one would assume if creativity were domain general.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号