全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1295篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
教育 | 1069篇 |
科学研究 | 56篇 |
各国文化 | 12篇 |
体育 | 33篇 |
文化理论 | 6篇 |
信息传播 | 131篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2022年 | 12篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 20篇 |
2019年 | 23篇 |
2018年 | 37篇 |
2017年 | 51篇 |
2016年 | 28篇 |
2015年 | 32篇 |
2014年 | 38篇 |
2013年 | 291篇 |
2012年 | 33篇 |
2011年 | 27篇 |
2010年 | 26篇 |
2009年 | 32篇 |
2008年 | 31篇 |
2007年 | 27篇 |
2006年 | 25篇 |
2005年 | 21篇 |
2004年 | 22篇 |
2003年 | 23篇 |
2002年 | 29篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 13篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 15篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 17篇 |
1980年 | 24篇 |
1979年 | 16篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 12篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1307条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
OBJECTIVE: This study compared the effectiveness of two types of instructor feedback (relative to no feedback) on investigative interviewers' ability to adhere to open-ended questions in simulated practice interviews about child abuse. METHOD: In one condition, feedback was provided at the end of each practice interview. In the other, the instructor stopped the interviewer at various stages during the practice interviews to provide feedback. The relative effect of these conditions was examined by measuring interviewers' performance in a standardized mock interview paradigm immediately prior to, immediately after, and 12 weeks after the practice and feedback sessions. RESULTS: Prior to and 12 weeks after the practice sessions were administered, there was no significant difference in participants' adherence to open-ended questions irrespective of the nature of the feedback, or whether feedback was received. At the immediate post-practice assessment interval, however, the participants who received feedback during the practice interviews performed better (M proportion of open-ended questions=.85, SD=.13) than the other participants (post-interview feedback M=.67, SD=.18, p<.001; no feedback M=.56, SD=.16, p<.001). This heightened use of open-ended questions was associated with a greater tendency among the interviewees to provide abuse-related details in response to open-ended questions (M=.91, SD=.11) compared to the other participants (post-interview feedback M=.77, SD=.15, p<.05; no feedback M=.69, SD=.16, p=.001). CONCLUSIONS: Different types of feedback can be differentially effective in training child abuse investigators to adhere to open-ended questions. The benefits of any training program, however, are likely to be short-lived without ongoing practice. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS: A single study compared the relative effectiveness of two types of instructor feedback (relative to no feedback) on investigative interviewers' ability to adhere to open-ended questions in simulated practice interviews about child abuse. This research is relevant to trainers of investigative interviewers because there is currently large variability in the type of feedback employed in training programs. This study is one of the first to empirically demonstrate that different types of feedback may be differentially effective in improving the performance of investigative interviewers. 相似文献
2.
This article explores the complex interplay of power between performativity and creativity agendas—a mutual tension that resides in British and Australian education. Accountability constraints and conflicting policy debates are problematised against the wider imperatives of similar government agendas. This ‘counterpoint’ of freedom and control has significant implications for pedagogy and, through accommodating performativity, teacher agency and professionalism are under threat. The authors propose a ‘rebalancing’ where pedagogy transforms from a site of struggle for control, to one where a higher trust is placed in teacher professionalism. The idea of ‘rebalancing pedagogy’ offers a way for teachers to navigate and be supported through the opposing demands of performativity and creativity. It acknowledges the importance of teacher agency and where teaching is judged against the characteristics of a systemic approach that facilitates the building of creative learning communities capable of supporting any curricula or content‐focused programmes in and beyond schools. 相似文献
3.
In 2 experiments we asked whether training in multiple contexts could eliminate context-dependent memory retrieval at 6 months as it does for adults. We found that 24-hour retention was disrupted when infants were trained in one context and tested in another but not when they were trained in multiple contexts prior to testing in a novel context (Experiment 1). After a long delay, however, training in multiple contexts did not facilitate memory retrieval in a novel context: An otherwise effective retrieval cue (the training mobile) did not alleviate forgetting 3 weeks later when it was presented in a novel context (Experiment 2). These findings demonstrate that multiple learning contexts can override the debilitating effects of an altered context on memory retrieval at 6 months, but only over the short term. The resistance of older memories to retrieval in novel contexts after long periods of disuse may be adaptive, insuring that potentially inappropriate or irrelevant memories will not be expressed. 相似文献
4.
Infants as young as 3 months of age can encode the relations among object features. Because object recognition depends critically upon a match between perceived feature configurations and representations of the object in long-term memory, the present experiments focused on infants' long-term memory for feature correlations. In 3 experiments with 72 3-month-olds, we documented the forgetting functions of different feature correlations, examined their relation to infants' memory for individual features, and replicated the findings with different stimuli. Infants were trained to activate a mobile composed of two kinds of blocks that differed in color, the figures displayed on them, and the figures' colors and were tested after different delays with recombinations of either the block colors, the figures, or the figure colors. Infants remembered some of the original feature combinations for up to 3 days but forgot all of them after 4 days. Even after 4 days, however, infants remembered the individual features that had entered into the original combinations. These results demonstrate that very young infants not only encode the relations among object features but also remember them for several days. Moreover, there is a dissociation in memory between features and feature relations: Feature relations are forgotten sooner than the individual features that comprise those relations. 相似文献
5.
William D. Eiserman Lenore Shisler Terry Foust Jan Buhrmann Randi Winston Karl R. White 《Early childhood research quarterly》2007
This study assessed the feasibility of doing hearing screening in Migrant, American Indian and Early Head Start programs using otoacoustic emissions (OAE) technology. Staff members were trained to screen 0–3-year-old children for hearing loss using hand-held OAE equipment and a multi-step screening and referral protocol. Of the 3486 children screened as a part of the study, 77% passed an OAE screening at the first step, 18% more passed an OAE screening at the second step, and 5% were ultimately referred for medical or audiological follow-up. Eighty children were identified as having a hearing loss or disorder of the outer, middle or inner ear requiring treatment. Of these 80, six had permanent bilateral or unilateral hearing loss. Although the protocol suggested that the multi-step screening procedure should be completed within a 4-week time period or less, analysis of the data showed that for children requiring more than an initial OAE screening, the length of time over which the screening was completed ranged from 7 to 12 weeks. The median time required to complete a single OAE screening session was 4 minutes per child. The results demonstrate that OAE screening of young children using this protocol is practical and effective. The implications for conducting periodic hearing screening throughout early childhood are discussed. 相似文献
6.
Karl R. White 《Early education and development》1993,4(4):346-358
Since the implementation of Public Law 99457, the number of children served by early intervention programs has increased substantially. This article describes the d e s of advocacy and science in contributing to that expansion. A biosocial systems model of early development is used to explain how comparative experimental research can help to improve the cost effectiveness of early intervention programs. Examples of the benefits of such research are taken from the other articles reported in this issue of Early Education and Development. 相似文献
7.
In this paper, the authors discuss two complementary life history research projects investigating prospective teachers' identities in relation to their race and social class. Drawing on the experiences of one student from each study, they show how both whites and students of color primarily thought about one another as “others”—people who fundamentally differed from one another and whom they understood through preconceived ideas about a group. The authors draw on a Bakhtinian notion of how a “surplus of sight” enables one to develop understandings of an “other” and also of oneself. 相似文献
8.
9.
Robyn McCarthy Carolyn Blackburn Raja Mukherjee Kate Fleming Clare Allely Lauren Kirby Penny A. Cook 《British Journal of Special Education》2022,49(1):41-63
Three of the most prevalent developmental disorders (DDs) are autism spectrum disorder (ASD), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FASD). As part of a study screening for DDs in Greater Manchester, UK, a unique opportunity was taken to explore parents' experiences of receiving reports about their child's previously unrecognised DD. Six parents out of a possible 16 took part in interviews, which were analysed thematically. Findings drawn from parental responses revealed a number of barriers to accessing support for their child's additional needs, including perceived resistance from schools, particularly for quiet, well-behaved girls, and difficulty in accessing assessment or referrals. There needs to be greater awareness of additional needs in children without externalising behaviours, the presence of gender-specific differences in the presentation of DDs, and FASD as a commonly occurring DD. Ultimately, better support for children with DDs would reduce the burden of unmet needs for the children and their families, and for wider services. 相似文献
10.
Peter T. White 《Research in Science Education》1982,12(1):97-101
Conclusion This study is preliminary in nature, and the instrument will require some modification to reduce its length and to further
increase the reliability of the attitude scale. None-the-less, in addition to replicating some important aspects of previous
studies, and introducing a measurement of primary school teachers' science related attitudes, it does provide valuable data
prior to the introduction of the new course guides. When used in conjunction with the findings of a similar survey which is
to be conducted in 1983, these data will help to assess the impact of the introductory phase of the course in Victorian Schools. 相似文献