首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   16630篇
  免费   245篇
  国内免费   121篇
教育   11468篇
科学研究   2096篇
各国文化   122篇
体育   1501篇
综合类   11篇
文化理论   129篇
信息传播   1669篇
  2021年   122篇
  2020年   233篇
  2019年   356篇
  2018年   491篇
  2017年   424篇
  2016年   432篇
  2015年   285篇
  2014年   354篇
  2013年   2919篇
  2012年   334篇
  2011年   377篇
  2010年   267篇
  2009年   279篇
  2008年   294篇
  2007年   323篇
  2006年   296篇
  2005年   304篇
  2004年   291篇
  2003年   268篇
  2002年   262篇
  2001年   272篇
  2000年   292篇
  1999年   285篇
  1998年   155篇
  1997年   166篇
  1996年   202篇
  1995年   181篇
  1994年   144篇
  1993年   155篇
  1992年   226篇
  1991年   231篇
  1990年   239篇
  1989年   264篇
  1988年   214篇
  1987年   230篇
  1986年   222篇
  1985年   251篇
  1984年   227篇
  1983年   217篇
  1982年   182篇
  1981年   172篇
  1980年   159篇
  1979年   246篇
  1978年   193篇
  1977年   188篇
  1976年   151篇
  1975年   135篇
  1974年   153篇
  1973年   132篇
  1971年   120篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 84 毫秒
941.
Effects of video-feedback on improvement of the tennis service were investigated in subjects having at least 2 years of playing experience in tennis. The experiment was carried out in an indoor tennis hall under normal training conditions. Subjects were randomly divided into three groups: a video-feedback training (VFT) group, a traditional training (TT) group and a control group, each consisting of 22 subjects. While the subjects of the control group received no training at all, subjects in both the VFT and TT groups were trained twice weekly during 5 consecutive weeks. Each training session lasted 40 min, of which 30 min was spent on actual practice in training of the service. The remaining 10 min was spent on watching, analysing and discussing video recordings of either their own service performed during the training session (VFT group), or ground strokes and volleys of top level players (TT group). Both the VFT and TT group showed significantly greater improvements in both achievement scores and form (technique) scores than did the control group. No differences, however, could be demonstrated between the VFT and TT group, indicating that the subjects of the former group did not benefit from the video-feedback they received. While form scores correlated significantly (P less than 0.001) with the velocity of the served ball, no such relation between form scores and spatial accuracy was apparent.  相似文献   
942.
We statistically controlled fitness (1.5-mile run), outcome-expectancy values, and perceived barriers for physical activity and then compared internal health locus of control (IHLOC) with internal exercise locus of control (IEXLOC) for predicting the physical activity of college students (N = 84). Prospective observations of self-reported free-living physical activity (seven-day recall) and supervised running (time x distance) were made at 2-, 5-, and 9-week intervals. IHLOC predicted seven-day recall at Week 2 (beta = .19) and Week 5 (beta = .36) (increase in adjusted R2 ranged from .05 to .12, p less than .05). Consistent with theory, the prediction was not reproducible when generalized outcome-expectancy value rather than outcome-expectancy value for health was assessed. IHLOC was unrelated to supervised running. IEXLOC was unrelated to activity in all analyses, even though exercise-specific measures of outcome-expectancy values (beta s = .20) and perceived barriers (beta s = .27-.32) predicted both seven-day recall and supervised running (p less than .05). IHLOC also discriminated (p less than .05) high active and low active subjects when criterion groups were formed from population norms on seven-day recall (greater than or less than 280 kcal.kg-1.week-1). Results indicate that tests of decision theories that include locus of control measures specific to health or exercise must adjust for fitness, perceived barriers to physical activity, and behaviorally relevant outcome-expectancy values of physical activity when studying college students. The construct validity of exercise locus of control remains uncertain for college students.  相似文献   
943.
944.
This article discusses a particular type of concordance table and the potential for test score misuse that may result from employing such a table. The concordance that is discussed is typically created between scores on different, nonequatable versions of a test that share the same or close to the same test title. These concordance tables often appear in the context of relating scores on computerized adaptive and paper‐and‐pencil versions of the same test. When such a table is presented in a complete point‐by‐point fashion, relating each reported score on the scale of the new version of the test to a reported score on the scale of the old version of the test, test score users will typically treat the table as if it represented an equating of scores between the two versions, and directly replace scores on the new version of the test by scores on the old version. This clearly represents a misuse of the test scores. Suggestions for avoiding this misuse of test scores from concordance tables are provided.  相似文献   
945.
946.
947.
948.
949.
950.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号