首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1861篇
  免费   48篇
  国内免费   3篇
教育   1545篇
科学研究   49篇
各国文化   34篇
体育   72篇
综合类   2篇
文化理论   4篇
信息传播   206篇
  2023年   7篇
  2022年   12篇
  2021年   21篇
  2020年   55篇
  2019年   47篇
  2018年   73篇
  2017年   113篇
  2016年   72篇
  2015年   58篇
  2014年   78篇
  2013年   406篇
  2012年   66篇
  2011年   63篇
  2010年   65篇
  2009年   54篇
  2008年   75篇
  2007年   48篇
  2006年   48篇
  2005年   47篇
  2004年   45篇
  2003年   32篇
  2002年   55篇
  2001年   32篇
  2000年   24篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   14篇
  1997年   26篇
  1996年   19篇
  1995年   22篇
  1994年   17篇
  1993年   17篇
  1992年   14篇
  1991年   20篇
  1990年   9篇
  1989年   13篇
  1988年   13篇
  1987年   15篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   13篇
  1984年   8篇
  1983年   10篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   6篇
  1980年   8篇
  1979年   8篇
  1978年   5篇
  1977年   10篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   6篇
  1971年   3篇
排序方式: 共有1912条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
221.
Given the dearth of high-quality curriculum materials aligned with the new standards (NGSS and CCSS) and low student persistence in STEM fields, we sought to develop and test a STEM curriculum that would improve student knowledge, interest, and emotions. A cluster randomized control trial was conducted to assess the impact of Speedometry, a two-unit STEM curriculum that uses familiar toy cars to explore math and science concepts. A total of 1,615 fourth-grade students across 48 classrooms and 17 schools in an urban district participated in the study. Using a 3-level multilevel model (students nested within teachers and schools) and controls for student and teacher characteristics, we found that Speedometry led to significant increases in student knowledge and positive emotions such as excitement. Speedometry also led to significant decreases in negative emotions such as boredom, frustration, and confusion. The curriculum was implemented with high fidelity as evidenced by classroom observations and teacher self-reports.  相似文献   
222.
223.
224.
225.
An analogy is a model drawn from one contextand used to support understanding in anothercontext. This study investigates the extent towhich analogies in instructional science booksare provided by authors. The books surveyed areavailable from commercial suppliers andintended for use in the elementary schoolclassroom with 7 to 11 year old children.Eighty texts were analysed for the presence orabsence of analogies. Forty-five of the booksoffered no analogies at all. In the remainingthirty-five books, 92 analogies were found.These were classified in line with earlier workon analogies for older students by Curtis andReigeluth (1984) and the findings arediscussed. The extent to which teachers candraw upon the analogies in such texts tosupport children's understanding isconsidered.  相似文献   
226.
227.
Induction programs are essential in addressing the unique pedagogical and content needs of secondary science teachers. Yet most secondary science teachers have little access to general induction programs, and even less opportunity to participate in specialized science induction programs. This study examines the impact of three different induction programs on secondary science teachers. The teachers were matched by grade level among three groups; one group participated in a science‐focused support program, another participated in a general support program, and a third had no formal support. All teachers were monitored during the spring semester to understand their teaching beliefs, instructional practices, and experiences in the classroom. The analysis of data revealed that the secondary science teachers in the science‐focused support program implemented more student‐centered inquiry lessons, held beliefs aligned with student‐centered practices, and felt fewer constraints in their teaching than did the other two groups of teachers. This study reinforces the importance of induction programs for teachers, suggests there is a need for specialized support programs for beginning science teachers, and recommends that universities and school districts work together to develop such programs. © 2003 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J Res Sci Teach 40: 77–97, 2003  相似文献   
228.
The rate at which 266 boys and girls ages 5 to 7 years old were victimized by peers was observed on multiple occasions in kindergarten and first grade. Individual differences in victimization were observed at kindergarten entry and in growth over the subsequent 2 years. Victimization increased for some children but decreased for others. Growth in victimization was reciprocally related to growth in teacher-reported antisocial and depressive behavior for boys. For girls, kindergarten victimization was related to growth in parent-reported antisocial behavior, teacher-reported depressive behavior to growth in victimization, and growth in victimization to parent-reported depression. At a short-term group level, antisocial behavior had a lagged suppressive effect on victimization for boys but a facilitating effect for girls.  相似文献   
229.
230.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号