首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   7224篇
  免费   66篇
  国内免费   6篇
教育   5354篇
科学研究   434篇
各国文化   127篇
体育   494篇
综合类   4篇
文化理论   82篇
信息传播   801篇
  2021年   50篇
  2020年   75篇
  2019年   131篇
  2018年   136篇
  2017年   197篇
  2016年   183篇
  2015年   119篇
  2014年   139篇
  2013年   1789篇
  2012年   161篇
  2011年   145篇
  2010年   129篇
  2009年   146篇
  2008年   175篇
  2007年   165篇
  2006年   147篇
  2005年   147篇
  2004年   143篇
  2003年   133篇
  2002年   127篇
  2001年   82篇
  2000年   96篇
  1999年   80篇
  1998年   77篇
  1997年   92篇
  1996年   88篇
  1995年   85篇
  1994年   92篇
  1993年   92篇
  1992年   82篇
  1991年   108篇
  1990年   84篇
  1989年   86篇
  1988年   86篇
  1987年   75篇
  1986年   81篇
  1985年   68篇
  1984年   72篇
  1983年   97篇
  1982年   72篇
  1981年   77篇
  1980年   78篇
  1979年   73篇
  1978年   94篇
  1977年   75篇
  1976年   65篇
  1975年   62篇
  1973年   47篇
  1970年   52篇
  1969年   45篇
排序方式: 共有7296条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
981.
Resume Dans cet article, nous travaillons sur le discours non strictement mathématique qu'un enseignant peut tenir en classe de mathématiques; nous nous sommes restreintes à des élèves de plus de 15 ans, et aux moments d'exposition des connaissances. Nous présentons d'abord trois grandes fonctions que l'enseignant peut donner à ce discours (communication, structuration et étiquetage, réflexion), en illustrant notre propos par des exemples empruntés à un même cours de niveau universitaire. Nous cherchons diverses caractéristiques de ces trois fonctions, en dégageant notamment leur lien avec le déroulement même du cours, leur portée éventuelle pour les étudiants, leur lien avec les contenus exposés... Nous en déduisons des pistes de recherches ultérieures, notamment en ce qui concerne la troisiéme fonction, qui nous semble la plus susceptible d'être l'objet d'un travail spécifique de préparation de l'enseignant.
Analysis of not strictly mathematical discourse in mathematics classes
In this article we examine the discourse which is not strictly mathematical that a teacher might adopt in a mathematics class. We have confined our discussion to students over 15 and to periods of teacher exposition. First we present three major functions of such discourse:communication, structuring and labelling, and reflection. We examine various characteristics of these three functions, primarily to make clear their relevance to the unfolding of the lesson itself, their influence on the students, and their bearing on the material taught, from this we develop lines for future enquiry, notably on the third function, which seems to us to be the most likely focus for specific preparation by the teacher.
  相似文献   
982.
In this article, I describe the 21 ideas underlying a 42-year search to understand giftedness. I present the ideas roughly chronologically, in the order in which they arose, and discuss how in a career as in science, progress means supplementing or even superseding one idea with the next. In terms of the 21 ideas, I start with a discussion of how I thought IQ tests could account for giftedness and end with a discussion of the ACCEL (Active Concerned Citizenship and Ethical Leadership) model. But I frame the article in terms of a paradox—that despite the fact that IQs rose 30 points during the 20th century, people often seem to be operating at an intellectual level that is not notably higher and may even be lower in some respects than in previous times.  相似文献   
983.
Kail R 《Child development》2002,73(6):1703-1714
The aim of the present research was to examine age-related change in proactive interference, which refers to impaired recall due to interference from material presented previously. Study 1 was a meta-analysis based on 26 studies that included 82 data sets. The results indicated that proactive interference decreased between 4 and 13 years of age. In Study 2, children from grades 3 through 6 and college students (N = 125) were administered a short-term memory task in which they briefly remembered sets of three words. For all ages, recall was accurate on Trial 1. However, recall became less accurate over Trials 2 through 4, particularly for the younger children in the sample. In addition, structural equation modeling revealed that age-related change in interference was linked to age-related change in speed of information processing. Results are discussed in terms of the nature of age-related change in interference.  相似文献   
984.
Designers of computer-based practice systems give primary attention to aspects of cognitive learning. However, observations of students practicing arithmetic, with two CAI systems widely used in Israel, revealed factors in the computer learning environment that substantially affected students' social behavior, particularly with respect to competition and cooperation in the course of learning. A questionnaire was designed on the basis of the observations to examine the effects of these factors in CAI work. It was administered to 457 fourth graders in 16 classes (two CAI systems x two SES categories x two schools x two classes). While neither CAI system was intended to foster competition, the observations and questionnaire data suggested that one encourages social comparison and competition whereas the other discourages these features and encourages cooperation and peer help. Boys appear to be significantly more competitive than girls. One system, compared with the other, significantly increases competitive behavior of high achievers (but not of low achievers); results in teachers' greater encouragement of social comparison and competition among students; and, as used in the schools we studied, causes disadvantaged students rather than advantaged students to develop negative feelings because of low CAI performance. The other system discourages competitive behavior and encourages cooperation. All these findings bear important implications for the instructional design of computer-based learning environments.  相似文献   
985.
986.
The history of science reveals a number of significant shifts in the conception of the universe. Recently, 20th century physics with its new discoveries has broken away from the traditional view of the universe to seeing it as interrelated, holistic, and emergent. This version of reality is complementary with the concept of dialectics. Dialectics is defined accordingly and its flexibility is suited to guide the present eclectic stance in counselling psychology and psychotherapy. Counselling psychology and psychotherapy are discussed within Kuhn's (1970) framework of normal science, crisis, and revolution. The normal science phase demonstrates variations of the subject/object dichotomy, and active/passive therapeutic orientations. Eclecticism reflects the crisis stage. Eclecticism is a multiple perspective that does not usually guide metatheory. The revolution and resolution phase for therapeutic psychology is manifested in a new paradigm based on dialectics. Dialectics makes eclecticism legitimate and comments towards that end are presented.  相似文献   
987.
Little responding develops to a conditioned stimulus (CS) that is placed in a random relation to an unconditioned stimulus (US). However, if the USs not preceded by that CS are themselves signaled by another stimulus, then the CS does come to elicit responding. This result has been attributed (e.g., by Durlach, 1983) to the signal’s blocking of conditioning to background cues that otherwise would prevent conditioning of the CS. However, Goddard and Jenkins (1987) have suggested the alternative that signaling the USs promotes responding due to the adventitious creation of periods of signaled nonreinforcement. Two experiments were conducted to assess this alternative, involving an autoshaping preparation in pigeons. In Experiment 1, little responding to a keylight CS presented in a random relation to a food US occurred, despite the explicit presentation of a discrete noise signaling periods of no food in the intertrial interval (ITI). Experiment 2 was designed to replicate the procedure of Goddard and Jenkins, in which an auditory stimulus extended throughout the ITI of a random schedule, terminating only prior to extra USs and during the CS. Contrary to their findings, little responding developed to the target CS. However, responding did develop when the sound-free period occurred only prior to the extra USs. These results offer little support for the hypothesis that signaled periods of nonreinforcement promote responding on random schedules. However, they are consistent with the view that signaling of ITI USs acts by preventing conditioning of potentially competitive background cues.  相似文献   
988.
Teaching Economics   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
989.
990.
The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of the age of viewers and the gender of the narrator in a film, and to examine the interaction between these two variables. Subjects in second and fifth grades were randomly assigned to two treatment groups. One group watched a film narrated by a female and the other watched the same film narrated by a male. As subjects watched the film, the experimenters measured their visual attention to the program. Recall of story ideas was also measured, using a multiple-choice test. The results indicated that the gender of the narrator influenced the recall of second-graders, but did not influence the recall of fifth-graders. Results suggest that older children pay greater visual attention to a program than younger children. Implications for developing audio-visual materials for children are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号