首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1810篇
  免费   5篇
  国内免费   37篇
教育   1186篇
科学研究   60篇
各国文化   4篇
体育   9篇
综合类   27篇
信息传播   566篇
  2023年   17篇
  2022年   37篇
  2021年   108篇
  2020年   73篇
  2019年   32篇
  2018年   37篇
  2017年   34篇
  2016年   20篇
  2015年   26篇
  2014年   120篇
  2013年   122篇
  2012年   116篇
  2011年   128篇
  2010年   70篇
  2009年   136篇
  2008年   123篇
  2007年   142篇
  2006年   85篇
  2005年   100篇
  2004年   61篇
  2003年   45篇
  2002年   47篇
  2001年   41篇
  2000年   33篇
  1999年   13篇
  1998年   9篇
  1997年   11篇
  1996年   14篇
  1995年   9篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   7篇
  1992年   10篇
  1991年   7篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   4篇
  1985年   1篇
  1983年   2篇
  1981年   1篇
排序方式: 共有1852条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
An unexpected and remarkable preference for second language reading among some dyslexics has been noted, presenting a challenge to accepted theory on dyslexia and the capacity for second language learning. The current study was designed to examine this phenomenon by systematically looking at the differential reading scores in the first and second languages of reading-disabled young Swedish adults who claimed to prefer reading in their second language (English). Three groups were selected for study: a group of 10 reading-disabled young adults who prefer to read English; a second group of 10 reading-disabled with no special preference for second language reading, matched on word recognition efficiency, age group, gender and educational level and a group of 10 normal readers matched on age group and educational level. The test battery was designed to compare overall reading efficiency in English and Swedish and therefore encompassed both speed and accuracy measures. The battery covered seven phonological measures, four orthographic measures,three isolated word reading measures, two continuous text reading measures, a comprehension task and an author recognition task. All tasks were carried out in both English and Swedish. The results showed that two dyslexic groups differed significantly in the degree to which task performance, including reading efficiency, was impeded by the English format. A tentative hypothesis was forwarded as to how the exceptional and unexpected facility with English might be explained.  相似文献   
52.
赵妍 《天津教育》2021,(11):24-25
《义务教育数学课程标准(2011版)》提出了十个核心词,“数感”是其中一个,且位列第一,是教师在备课、研课、评课时常用的关键词。关于数感及学生数感的培养,许多专家、学者、一线教师已有了丰硕的理论及实践研究结果。相比之下,小学低年级的研究较之中高年级则单薄得多。然而数感作为一种自觉的行为,更应该从学生起步阶段加以培养,应渗透到数学学习的整个过程。  相似文献   
53.
陈立永 《天津教育》2021,(3):141-142
在初中语文教学过程中,只有巩固学生阅读和写作能力,才能更好地在原有的基础上提高学生的综合语文素养。为此,在进行语文教学过程中,教师只有强调读写结合教学方法运用的有效性,才能从根本上提升学生的写作能力,才能更好地提高教学质量。本文在此基础上,深入探讨初中语文教学中读写结合教学方式的具体应用以及相应的应用策略。  相似文献   
54.
A flash card computer program designed to increase decoding speed in reading was evaluated with a sample of 55 eight- to twelve-year-old Dutch poor readers by using a pretest-training-posttest control group design. The reading level of the poor readers was comparable to the reading level of normal readers in grade two. After 8 weeks of practicing (twice a week) for 30 minutes per session, children in the training group showed substantial improvements in decoding speed not only on pseudowords directly practiced during the training, but also in untrained pseudowords and in existing words. The no-training control group did not improve in decoding speed from pre- to posttest. The results, furthermore, indicate that syllable-bound processes play a role in phonological decoding in Dutch, and that the flash card method used in this study stimulates poor readers to decode letter strings into syllabic units, which leads to an increase in decoding speed. The role of the syllable in phonological decoding and the potential of a specific flash card technique as a means to improve decoding skills of poor readers are discussed.  相似文献   
55.
Søvik  Nils  Arntzen  Oddvar  Samuelstuen  Marit 《Reading and writing》2000,13(3-4):237-255
This study addressed the relationship between four eyemovement parameters and reading speed of 20twelve-year-old children during silent and oralreading. The results indicated that each of theparameters correlated significantly with speed ofsilent reading. In fact, reading speed could bepredicted by the following variables: recognitionspan, average fixation duration, and number ofregressive saccades. Moreover, the relationshipbetween silent and oral reading speed, on one hand,and between reading fluency and reading errors in oralreading speed, on the other, was examined. The resultsindicated that in terms of reading speed, significantinterrelationships existed between silent and oralreading. Furthermore, fluency and errors wereintercorrelated phenomena, but only fluency wassubstantially related to oral reading speed.  相似文献   
56.
阅读教学是大学英语教学的重点之一,而培养学生的自主阅读能力应该是大学英语阅读教学的重要组成部分。本文针对大学生阅读教学中存在的问题,系统地提出培养自主阅读能力的方法。  相似文献   
57.
Interest in an expository text: How does it flow from reading to writing?   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
This study investigated the flow of interest in a reading-to-write activity. It was hypothesized that (a) different contents in a text would stimulate different types of interest and (b) different writing tasks would impact differently students’ use of interesting text segments. Participants were 247 11th- and 12th-grade students, who were divided into 6 groups according to a 2 (interesting vs. uninteresting topic) × 3 (type of writing task) design. While reading, students rated novelty, curiosity, impressiveness, importance, and willingness to reflect on each text segment. Participants were then assigned to one of three writing tasks, namely argumentation, text designing, and composition. Results showed that concepts were regarded as important, events/activities as impressive, and evaluations as most worthy of reflection. Topic interest was explicitly expressed mainly in the argumentation condition, where increased topic interest scores after reading and writing were also found.  相似文献   
58.
This research aimed at identifying unique effects of reading motivation on reading performance when controlling for cognitive skills, familial, and demographic background. We drew upon a longitudinal sample of N = 1508 secondary school students from 5th to 8th grade. Two types of intrinsic reading motivation (reading enjoyment, reading for interest), one type of extrinsic reading motivation (competition), and reading self-concept were measured by self-report questionnaires. Cognitive skills (reasoning, decoding speed) and reading performance were assessed using standardized tests and background variables were collected using student and parent questionnaires. Applying latent growth curve modeling, positive unique effects of reading enjoyment and reading self-concept and a negative unique effect of competition on the initial level of reading performance were recorded. Moreover, a positive unique effect of reading for interest on reading performance growth was recorded. One may conclude that enhancing students’ interest might be fruitful in terms of nurturing reading performance.  相似文献   
59.
目前小学语文阅读中开放性题目的设计及教学对情感态度价值观目标的实现并不是很理想。新课标要求"培养学生高尚的道德情操和健康的审美情趣,形成正确的价值观和积极的人生态度,是语文教学的重要内容",但是小学语文阅读中存在着阅读文本陈旧、提问方式单一、讲解刻板等问题。所以在阅读教学中需要教科书编写者及教师共合作让阅读文本生活化、问题设计多样化,问题呈现情境化及教师鼓励多样化。同时家庭、学校和社会共同合作帮助学生树立正确价值观。  相似文献   
60.
According to the Simple View of Reading (SVR), reading comprehension is the product of word decoding ability and linguistic comprehension (R = D × C). However, there is also evidence showing that an additive model (R = D + C) explains just as much or even more of the variance in reading comprehension than the product model. To further evaluate these models, 467 Norwegian students in grade 6 were administered measures of reading comprehension, decoding ability, listening comprehension, phonemic awareness, and rapid digit naming. Hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to evaluate the explanatory power of each model, in addition to the influence of phonemic awareness and rapid naming. The preliminary hierarchical regression analyses were replicated among Swedish students. According to the present study, the additive model explained more of the variance in reading comprehension than the product model, and only rapid naming contributed significantly to explain variance in reading comprehension, above and beyond that explained by the SVR.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号