全文获取类型
收费全文 | 313篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
教育 | 269篇 |
科学研究 | 28篇 |
体育 | 19篇 |
综合类 | 6篇 |
文化理论 | 1篇 |
信息传播 | 4篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 3篇 |
2017年 | 5篇 |
2016年 | 5篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 8篇 |
2013年 | 19篇 |
2012年 | 18篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 14篇 |
2009年 | 21篇 |
2008年 | 19篇 |
2007年 | 21篇 |
2006年 | 18篇 |
2005年 | 10篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 13篇 |
2002年 | 19篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有327条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
As a wide range of values has been reported for the relative energetics of 400-m and 800-m track running events, this study aimed to quantify the respective aerobic and anaerobic energy contributions to these events during track running. Sixteen trained 400-m (11 males, 5 females) and 11 trained 800-m (9 males, 2 females) athletes participated in this study. The participants performed (on separate days) a laboratory graded exercsie test and multiple race time-trials. The relative energy system contribution was calculated by multiple methods based upon measures of race [Vdot]O2, accumulated oxygen deficit (AOD), blood lactate and estimated phosphocreatine degradation (lactate/PCr). The aerobic/anaerobic energy system contribution (AOD method) to the 400-m event was calculated as 41/59% (male) and 45/55% (female). For the 800-m event, an increased aerobic involvement was noted with a 60/40% (male) and 70/30% (female) respective contribution. Significant (P <?0.05) negative correlations were noted between race performance and anaerobic energy system involvement (lactate/PCr) for the male 800-m and female 400-m events (r = ? 0.77 and ??0.87 respectively). These track running data compare well with previous estimates of the relative energy system contributions to the 400-m and 800-m events. Additionally, the relative importance and speed of interaction of the respective metabolic pathways has implications to training for these events. 相似文献
82.
83.
The purpose of this study was to investigate whether naming speed makes a contribution to the prediction of reading comprehension,
after taking into account the product of word decoding and listening comprehension (i.e., the Simple View of Reading; [Gough,
P.B. & Tunmer, W.E. (1986). Remedial and Special Education 7, 6–10]), and phonological awareness. In grade 3, word decoding was measured with the Woodcock [(1998). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests – Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services]. Word Identification and Word Attack subtests, listening comprehension with
the Woodcock (1991) [Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery – Revised. Chicago: Riverside Publishing Company] test of Listening Comprehension, naming speed with a picture naming task, and 4 measures
assessed phonological awareness. Reading comprehension was assessed in grades 3, 4, and 5 with the Woodcock (1998) Passage
Comprehension subtest and in grade 5 with the Gates–MacGinitie reading test. The Simple View was evaluated twice: first, with
a pseudoword measure for decoding (Grapheme–Phoneme-conversion product) and, second, with a word identification measure for
decoding (word recognition product). Hierarchical regression and commonality analyses indicated that the decoding and listening
comprehension products accounted for considerable variance in reading comprehension. Naming speed had a small but significant
effect after accounting for the Grapheme–Phoneme-conversion product (2–3%), but little effect after accounting for the word-recognition
product (0–2%). Subgroup analyses indicated that naming speed had its primary effect for less able readers. Commonality analyses
supported the interpretation that naming speed contributes after the Grapheme–Phoneme-conversion product but not after the
word recognition product because naming speed has already had its effect upon word recognition. These results indicate that
it is important how the Simple View decoding term is defined, and that the Simple View may be incomplete, especially for less able readers. 相似文献
84.
To examine the association between speech production and early literacy skills, this study of 102 preschool children looked at phonological awareness in relation to whether children were delayed, typical, or advanced in their articulation of consonants. Using a developmental typology inspired by some of the literature on speech development (Kahn and Lewis, The Kahn-Lewis phonological analysis, 1986; Shriberg, Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36(1):105-140, 1993a), we found that failure to master the early-8 consonants and a greater prevalence of certain types of production errors were associated with deficient phonological awareness. We also found that children who made no consonant errors had advanced phonological awareness relative to other children in the sample. In all cases, both productive speech patterns and speech errors were more closely linked with rhyme awareness than with phoneme awareness. The association between speech production and rhyme awareness may provide some new directions for the early preschool assessment of risk for reading problems. 相似文献
85.
We investigated the initial development of reading and spelling in European Portuguese. First-graders, tested in February
and June, had to read and spell words and pseudowords. In February there were regularity and graphemic complexity effects,
indicating that these children relied on grapheme–phoneme conversion. The lexicality effect found in spelling, in June, suggest
that by the end of first grade these children had begun to construct an orthographic lexicon. However, lexical addressing
is not inconsistent with phonological mediation as regularization errors increased between the sessions. Additionally, the
previously reported similarity in global performance of Portuguese and French beginning readers may conceal processing differences
that are related to specific characteristics of the corresponding orthographic codes.
相似文献
Sandra FernandesEmail: |
86.
运用生态足迹法对喀什地区的生态承载力进行分析,得出喀什地区的人均生态承载力为1.26954 hm2,人均生态足迹足迹为4.4001 hm2,人均生态赤字为3.1305 hm2,喀什地区人均生态足迹需求远远高于人均生态承载力,生态赤字严重化,对此提出提高土地单位面积的产量,调整该区域进出口贸易等对策. 相似文献
87.
通过分析当前专利质量问题的表现及成因,认为专利质量问题的实质是,大量的问题专利的存在导致大量的专利效力不确定及其引发的有关问题。因此,提高专利质量、解决专利质量问题就是减少不当授权,减少专利效力的不确定性。无效宣告、法院诉讼等事后解决专利效力的方式并不十分有效,而决定专利效力的关键环节———专利审查才是解决问题的根本所在。因此,应进一步加大审查投入,克服专利审查中信息不足和对专利申请人的申请行为进行有效规制。 相似文献
88.
生态足迹模型是度量可持续发展的一种方法,运用它对吉林省可持续发展状况的评价和未来发展趋势进行研究,为吉林省可持续发展提供决策依据。利用生态足迹模型研究了吉林省1978-2002年生态足迹和生态承载力,运用回归分析对吉林省2004-2016年生态足迹和生态承载力做出了预测。结果表明:1978-2002年,吉林省人均生态足迹从1.090 hm2增加到2.457 hm2,年平均增长率达到3.45%;人均生态承载力从1.210 hm2下降到1.096hm2,年均下降率为0.4%。除1978、1980年存在生态盈余,其余年份均是生态赤字,且生态赤字总体呈扩大趋势。2004-2016年,吉林省人均生态足迹年平均增长率将达到1.99%,人均生态承载力下降率达0.5%。探讨了生态足迹模型存在的一些问题以及尚需研究的内容。 相似文献
89.
Eleni Morfidi Aryan van der Leij Peter F. de Jong Femke Scheltinga Judith Bekebrede 《Reading and writing》2007,20(8):753-784
The present study investigated the reading of secondary school students in their first and second language (L1, L2). Twenty-six average and twenty-six poor readers matched on age, gender, listening and reading comprehension participated. They were native Dutch speakers who started learning English at secondary school (grade 7). We examined whether differences in L2 between the two groups reflect differences in L1 with regard to reading and relevant subskills. In addition, the relationship between reading and its predictors within and across the two languages was investigated. Between group differences were similar in L1 and L2 when task conditions involved high levels of phonological and orthographic complexity or demanded speeded processing. Furthermore, serial rapid naming predicted speeded word reading in both languages and L2 text reading accuracy, while L2 phoneme awareness and orthographic knowledge explained unique variance in L2 text reading accuracy. Cross-linguistic prediction revealed that speeded word reading predicted its counterpart from L1 to L2 and vice versa. Serial rapid naming explained additional variance in the prediction of L2 from L1. After exclusion of the reading predictor from the model, serial rapid naming was the most consistent cross-linguistic predictor, while L2 orthographic knowledge explained a small amount of unique variance in L1 speeded word reading. 相似文献
90.
Recent evidence suggests that training in phoneme awareness has a positive impact on beginning reading and spelling. The objective of this study was to investigate the effectiveness of instruction in phonological awareness provided in low-income, inner-city kindergarten classrooms by kindergarten teachers and their teaching assistants. Prior to the intervention, the 84 treatment children and 75 control children, who attended inner-city schools in an urban district in upstate New York, did not differ on age, sex, race, SES, PPVT-R score, phoneme segmentation, letter name knowledge, letter sound knowledge, or reading. After the 11 week intervention, the treatment children significantly outperformed the control children on measures of phoneme segmentation, letter name and letter sound knowledge, two of three reading measures, and a measure of invented spelling. Implications for improving beginning reading instruction are discussed. 相似文献