首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   9886篇
  免费   56篇
  国内免费   78篇
教育   7197篇
科学研究   827篇
各国文化   6篇
体育   98篇
综合类   353篇
文化理论   8篇
信息传播   1531篇
  2024年   2篇
  2023年   48篇
  2022年   95篇
  2021年   297篇
  2020年   364篇
  2019年   208篇
  2018年   127篇
  2017年   133篇
  2016年   134篇
  2015年   280篇
  2014年   715篇
  2013年   838篇
  2012年   950篇
  2011年   919篇
  2010年   547篇
  2009年   511篇
  2008年   533篇
  2007年   613篇
  2006年   571篇
  2005年   503篇
  2004年   434篇
  2003年   363篇
  2002年   281篇
  2001年   232篇
  2000年   140篇
  1999年   35篇
  1998年   35篇
  1997年   21篇
  1996年   20篇
  1995年   7篇
  1994年   15篇
  1993年   8篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   11篇
  1990年   10篇
  1989年   13篇
  1957年   3篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
纳米线阵列结构温差电材料具有高的热电转换效率 ,非常适合制造输出功率在微瓦量级的微温差电池 .由于纳米线阵列结构温差电材料的厚度通常在十微米至几十微米 ,常规的温差电性能测试技术已不适用 .为此 ,提出了一种新的温差电性能测试技术 ,并建立了相关的测试系统 .  相似文献   
82.
充分利用各种现代教学设备在语言实验室进行英语教学,可以大大提高学生的英语听说能力、语言综合运用能力和语言交际能力,同时也能培养学生的观察、记忆、想象能力和创新精神,从而优化英语教学效果。  相似文献   
83.
谈语文的阅读与写作教学   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
通过课文教给学生写作的方法、技巧,通过一堂课让学生有所得。通过指导学生阅读、分析课文,帮助学生学会这篇文章的写作方法或特色,并能运用到写作实践中去。  相似文献   
84.
针对高职院校日语专业的学生听说能力提高较慢的现状,提出了日语教学应以听力教学为重心,运用思维跟随方法,实施精听精解的教学模式,以期使学生尽快适应日语的思维和表达特点,有效地提高听力及会话能力。  相似文献   
85.
高校教材的集成化管理   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
高校教材管理作为教学管理中的一项重要内容,涉及了多方面的具体工作。把集成管理的思想引入教材管理工作中,通过管理的规范化、教材计划的统一协调、教材的合理选编与供应、教材的经济性等管理来实现新时期教材集成化管理,提高教材管理水平,更好地服务教学。  相似文献   
86.
试谈《二十一世纪报》阅读课程创新教学   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
英语报刊阅读作为一种新型的英语教学方式被引入课堂,必将丰富教学资源,拓宽学习和运用英语的渠道。以新课程标准为指导,在运用英语报刊进行英语阅读教学中,应综合应用听、说、读、写各项技能,以实现英语阅读教学的创新目标。  相似文献   
87.
提高大学俄语阅读能力的实践与思考   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
阅读能力的培养是大学俄语教学的重要任务。培养学生的阅读能力应贯穿于教学过程的始终,需要教师与学生紧密配合才能有效完成,二者缺一不可。  相似文献   
88.
Evidence strongly suggests that shared book reading at home and in preschool is important for young children's development of the foundational skills required for the eventual mastery of decoding and comprehension. Yet the nuances of how learning from book reading might vary across these contexts and with children's skills are not well understood. One hundred and thirty children participating in a longitudinal investigation of literacy development were videotaped reading a storybook with a parent. Children were also videotaped in their 33 preschool classrooms during the instructional book-reading portion of the day. Readings were coded for adult and child contextualized and decontextualized language relating to both decoding and meaning-making skills, and relations between this talk and emergent literacy outcomes were analyzed. Results demonstrate that parents and teachers overwhelmingly focus their book-related talk on meaning-related rather than code-related information, and that the relations between outcomes and talk depend in part on children's initial levels of vocabulary skills. Implications for practice and research are discussed.  相似文献   
89.
The present study examines the development of sight word reading in young children by examining changes in their self‐reported reading strategies over time. A sample of 65 five to seven year olds were asked to read 40 real word items, all carefully matched for letter length and word frequency, on three separate occasions. Changes in the children’s word identifications were measured using immediately retrospective verbal self‐reports. Overall, the results showed some variability in the children’s self‐reported reading strategies. Over time the children relied less on phonological strategies and moved towards reliance on directly retrieving words from memory. This change was most evident in the older children; while both year groups showed similar patterns of shifting reliance from explicit phonological strategies to retrieval, this shift was simply less frequent among the younger children. An analysis of word‐specific changes in reading strategies showed that the older children had a better sight vocabulary for more complex word items. These findings provide further support for Ehri’s mediated phase theory in explaining children’s development in learning to read.  相似文献   
90.
This paper reports on two studies which investigated the relationship between children's texting behaviour, their knowledge of text abbreviations and their school attainment in written language skills. In Study One, 11–12‐year‐old children provided information on their texting behaviour. They were also asked to translate a standard English sentence into a text message and vice versa. The children's standardised verbal and non‐verbal reasoning scores were also obtained. Children who used their mobiles to send three or more text messages a day had significantly lower scores than children who sent none. However, the children who, when asked to write a text message, showed greater use of text abbreviations (‘textisms’) tended to have better performance on a measure of verbal reasoning ability, which is highly associated with Key Stage 2 (KS2) and 3 English scores. In Study Two, children's performance on writing measures was examined more specifically. Ten to eleven‐year‐old children were asked to complete another English to text message translation exercise. Spelling proficiency was also assessed, and KS2 Writing scores were obtained. Positive correlations between spelling ability and performance on the translation exercise were found, and group‐based comparisons based on the children's writing scores also showed that good writing attainment was associated with greater use of textisms, although the direction of this association is nor clear. Overall, these findings suggest that children's knowledge of textisms is not associated with poor written language outcomes for children in this age range.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号