全文获取类型
收费全文 | 17820篇 |
免费 | 139篇 |
国内免费 | 38篇 |
专业分类
教育 | 13154篇 |
科学研究 | 1203篇 |
各国文化 | 189篇 |
体育 | 1247篇 |
综合类 | 44篇 |
文化理论 | 83篇 |
信息传播 | 2077篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 112篇 |
2021年 | 196篇 |
2020年 | 275篇 |
2019年 | 432篇 |
2018年 | 516篇 |
2017年 | 562篇 |
2016年 | 522篇 |
2015年 | 341篇 |
2014年 | 466篇 |
2013年 | 3533篇 |
2012年 | 464篇 |
2011年 | 477篇 |
2010年 | 422篇 |
2009年 | 415篇 |
2008年 | 433篇 |
2007年 | 411篇 |
2006年 | 434篇 |
2005年 | 377篇 |
2004年 | 267篇 |
2003年 | 260篇 |
2002年 | 246篇 |
2001年 | 385篇 |
2000年 | 297篇 |
1999年 | 266篇 |
1998年 | 166篇 |
1997年 | 164篇 |
1996年 | 185篇 |
1995年 | 156篇 |
1994年 | 169篇 |
1993年 | 148篇 |
1992年 | 234篇 |
1991年 | 224篇 |
1990年 | 245篇 |
1989年 | 230篇 |
1988年 | 191篇 |
1987年 | 190篇 |
1986年 | 195篇 |
1985年 | 181篇 |
1984年 | 191篇 |
1983年 | 171篇 |
1982年 | 144篇 |
1981年 | 139篇 |
1980年 | 136篇 |
1979年 | 197篇 |
1978年 | 166篇 |
1977年 | 124篇 |
1976年 | 126篇 |
1975年 | 114篇 |
1973年 | 110篇 |
1971年 | 118篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 17 毫秒
1.
How has Item Response Theory helped solve problems in the development and use of computer-adaptive tests? Do we need to balance item content with computer-adaptive tests? Could we use IRT to evaluate unusual responses to computer-delivered tests? 相似文献
2.
3.
Audrey L. Qualls 《Educational Measurement》2001,20(1):9-16
What is the typical amount of student answer‐changing that takes place in a low‐stakes test environment? Can we use this information to look for evidence of improper answer‐changing (cheating) on high‐stakes test? 相似文献
4.
L. Girolametto S. Bonifacio C. Visini E. Weitzman E. Zocconi P.S. Pearce 《International Journal of Early Years Education》2003,11(3):255-260
The aim was to examine cross-cultural variation in linguistic responsiveness to young children in 10 English-speaking mother–child dyads and 10 Italian-speaking mother– child dyads. All 20 children were late talkers who possessed delays in expressive vocabulary development but age-appropriate cognitive and receptive language skills. Dyads were filmed in 15 minute free play contexts, which were transcribed and coded for measures of maternal linguistic input (e.g. rate, MLU, labels, expansions) and child language productivity (e.g. utterances, different words used). The results revealed that the Italian mothers used more utterances, spoke more quickly and used a more diverse vocabulary than the Canadian mothers. The Italian children mirrored their mothers and also used more utterances and a more diverse vocabulary than the Canadian children. Mothers in both groups used similar percentages of responsive labels and expansions. However, Italian mothers responded to fewer of their children's vocalisations, using a smaller percentage of imitations and interpretations than the Canadian mothers. Correlations between maternal input and children's language productivity revealed that contingent language measures (e.g. imitations, interpretations, expansions) were related to high levels of productivity in children in both cultural groups. The results support the use of language interventions based on increasing maternal responsiveness for these children at the one-word stage of language development. They also point to differences that may be culturally based. For example, Italian mothers use faster rates of interaction and appear to have higher expectations for their children's verbal participation in interaction. This is reflected in higher rates of language production from their children, even though children in both cultural groups have similar vocabulary sizes. 相似文献
5.
A discussion of the influences of impulsivity on test performance is presented, along with suggested procedures for ascertaining more accurately the cognitive abilities of impulsive children. 相似文献
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
Triin Rääsk Kenn Konstabel Jarek Mäestu Evelin Lätt Toivo Jürimäe Jaak Jürimäe 《Journal of sports sciences》2015,33(16):1649-1657
We examined the tracking of physical activity (PA) measured by accelerometers and subjective self- and parental reports in normal weight and overweight and obese pubertal boys over two-year period. In total, 156 boys with mean (±SD) age of 11.53 ± 0.76 at baseline and with mean age of 13.94 ± 0.74 at 2 year follow-up were studied. At baseline and approximately two years later, the boys completed self-report questionnaire and wore an accelerometer for seven consecutive days. On the basis of first year assessment’s body mass index (BMI), the children were grouped as normal weight and overweight and obese groups according to BMI cut-offs. Tracking correlations of objectively measured PA and subjectively measured PA were fairly similar across the 12–14 year-old-boys weight groups over two year period. Tracking correlations of objectively measured PA and subjectively measured PA were not significantly different over two-year period between both BMI groups. The results of the study show that pubertal boys objectively measured PA decreased over two-year period and so the boys started to be less active in their pubertal period. 相似文献