首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   17820篇
  免费   139篇
  国内免费   38篇
教育   13154篇
科学研究   1203篇
各国文化   189篇
体育   1247篇
综合类   44篇
文化理论   83篇
信息传播   2077篇
  2022年   112篇
  2021年   196篇
  2020年   275篇
  2019年   432篇
  2018年   516篇
  2017年   562篇
  2016年   522篇
  2015年   341篇
  2014年   466篇
  2013年   3533篇
  2012年   464篇
  2011年   477篇
  2010年   422篇
  2009年   415篇
  2008年   433篇
  2007年   411篇
  2006年   434篇
  2005年   377篇
  2004年   267篇
  2003年   260篇
  2002年   246篇
  2001年   385篇
  2000年   297篇
  1999年   266篇
  1998年   166篇
  1997年   164篇
  1996年   185篇
  1995年   156篇
  1994年   169篇
  1993年   148篇
  1992年   234篇
  1991年   224篇
  1990年   245篇
  1989年   230篇
  1988年   191篇
  1987年   190篇
  1986年   195篇
  1985年   181篇
  1984年   191篇
  1983年   171篇
  1982年   144篇
  1981年   139篇
  1980年   136篇
  1979年   197篇
  1978年   166篇
  1977年   124篇
  1976年   126篇
  1975年   114篇
  1973年   110篇
  1971年   118篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 17 毫秒
1.
How has Item Response Theory helped solve problems in the development and use of computer-adaptive tests? Do we need to balance item content with computer-adaptive tests? Could we use IRT to evaluate unusual responses to computer-delivered tests?  相似文献   
2.
3.
What is the typical amount of student answer‐changing that takes place in a low‐stakes test environment? Can we use this information to look for evidence of improper answer‐changing (cheating) on high‐stakes test?  相似文献   
4.
The aim was to examine cross-cultural variation in linguistic responsiveness to young children in 10 English-speaking mother–child dyads and 10 Italian-speaking mother– child dyads. All 20 children were late talkers who possessed delays in expressive vocabulary development but age-appropriate cognitive and receptive language skills. Dyads were filmed in 15 minute free play contexts, which were transcribed and coded for measures of maternal linguistic input (e.g. rate, MLU, labels, expansions) and child language productivity (e.g. utterances, different words used). The results revealed that the Italian mothers used more utterances, spoke more quickly and used a more diverse vocabulary than the Canadian mothers. The Italian children mirrored their mothers and also used more utterances and a more diverse vocabulary than the Canadian children. Mothers in both groups used similar percentages of responsive labels and expansions. However, Italian mothers responded to fewer of their children's vocalisations, using a smaller percentage of imitations and interpretations than the Canadian mothers. Correlations between maternal input and children's language productivity revealed that contingent language measures (e.g. imitations, interpretations, expansions) were related to high levels of productivity in children in both cultural groups. The results support the use of language interventions based on increasing maternal responsiveness for these children at the one-word stage of language development. They also point to differences that may be culturally based. For example, Italian mothers use faster rates of interaction and appear to have higher expectations for their children's verbal participation in interaction. This is reflected in higher rates of language production from their children, even though children in both cultural groups have similar vocabulary sizes.  相似文献   
5.
A discussion of the influences of impulsivity on test performance is presented, along with suggested procedures for ascertaining more accurately the cognitive abilities of impulsive children.  相似文献   
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
We examined the tracking of physical activity (PA) measured by accelerometers and subjective self- and parental reports in normal weight and overweight and obese pubertal boys over two-year period. In total, 156 boys with mean (±SD) age of 11.53 ± 0.76 at baseline and with mean age of 13.94 ± 0.74 at 2 year follow-up were studied. At baseline and approximately two years later, the boys completed self-report questionnaire and wore an accelerometer for seven consecutive days. On the basis of first year assessment’s body mass index (BMI), the children were grouped as normal weight and overweight and obese groups according to BMI cut-offs. Tracking correlations of objectively measured PA and subjectively measured PA were fairly similar across the 12–14 year-old-boys weight groups over two year period. Tracking correlations of objectively measured PA and subjectively measured PA were not significantly different over two-year period between both BMI groups. The results of the study show that pubertal boys objectively measured PA decreased over two-year period and so the boys started to be less active in their pubertal period.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号